Together with the aforementioned studies by Kittler et al (2005)

Together with the aforementioned studies by Kittler et al. (2005), these findings indicate that phosphorylation of a single site in GABAAR β selleck chemicals subunits can have different effects on trafficking

of GABAARs depending on the kinase involved, most likely reflecting different subcellular compartments where phosphorylation of GABAARs occurs. Interestingly, Fujii et al. (2010) found that the effects of insulin on GABA-evoked currents are absent in neurons from PRIP1/2 double knockout mice. PRIP1 interacts with Akt directly and is required for insulin-induced association of Akt with GABAARs. Thus, PRIP serves as a multifunctional adaptor for both kinases and phosphatases and thereby contributes to both regulated membrane translocation and endocytosis of GABAARs (Fujii et al., 2010). Interestingly, insulin-mediated activation of Akt further results Enzalutamide supplier in inhibitory serine-phosphorylation of GSK3β (Cross et al., 1995). GSK3β in turn promotes postsynaptic GABAAR clustering and mIPSCs by reducing calpain-1-mediated cleavage of gephyrin (Tyagarajan et al., 2011) (Figures 5A and 6A). A second proposed mechanism involves direct interaction of the insulin receptor target PI3K with GABAARs (Figure 6B) (Vetiska et al., 2007). The PI3K-GABAAR complex was found to be present constitutively in brain

tissue, presumably in an intracellular inactive state. When brain slices were treated with insulin the abundance of this complex rapidly increased as well as its association with phosphorylated membrane lipids. This suggests that the complex is translocated to the plasma membrane in concert with PI3K-mediated phosphorylation of lipids. In vitro analyses revealed that the PI3K-GABAAR complex involves binding of the PI3K p85 subunit SH2 domain to phospho-tyrosines

(Tyr 372/379) in the intracellular loop region of β subunits. These Tyr residues were essential for insulin-induced surface expression of β2-containing receptors in transfected neurons. However, several aspects of this mechanism remain to be resolved. First, it is unclear whether this mechanism applies to GABAARs independently of the type of β subunit. Second, it is not known whether Tyr phosphorylation of β subunits is itself modulated by insulin, which Tyr kinase is involved in β subunit phosphorylation, and whether interaction of PI3K with β subunit STK38 phospho-tyrosines contributes to activation of PI3K enzyme function. Lastly, it is not known whether and how insulin-induced interaction between PI3K and GABAARs corroborates with the aforementioned Akt- and PRIP-dependent downstream effects of PI3K. Signaling by BDNF and its cognate receptor (receptor tropomyosin-related kinase B, TrkB) is critically important for neurogenesis (Bergami et al., 2008 and Li et al., 2008) and inhibitory synapse formation (Seil, 2003). BDNF is further involved in structural and functional neuronal plasticity in the adult nervous system (Xu et al., 2000).

, 2004; Mölle et al , 2006; Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al

, 2004; Mölle et al., 2006; Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al., 2003). Further strengthening the link, sharp waves in hippocampus have recently been

shown to be correlated with memory replay in mPFC (Peyrache et al., 2009). The directionality find more of the mPFC-hippocampal interaction during sleep has been difficult to discern, with some results suggesting events in hippocampus precede those in cortex (Battaglia et al., 2004; Wierzynski et al., 2009) while others suggest the opposite (Isomura et al., 2006; Mölle et al., 2006). Perhaps both sides are correct. It has been suggested that cortical events initiate hippocampal replay, which in turn reinforces the ongoing replay of patterns in the neocortex (Sirota et al., 2003). Alternatively, the directionality of information flow from hippocampus to mPFC may depend on whether the information being processed is newly learned. One puzzling aspect of these consolidation findings is that tasks affected by posttask mPFC disruption are

not necessarily mPFC-dependent during initial learning. For example, odor-reward selleck screening library associations tested 48 hr after learning are impaired by consolidation block, yet rats with prelimbic lesions can easily acquire and retrieve odor-reward associations within a single session (Birrell and Brown, 2000; Tronel and Sara, 2003). Likewise, a NMDA antagonist injected into infralimbic cortex after extinction training interferes with the consolidation of fear extinction but NMDA receptor block during extinction training has no effect on within-session acquisition of extinction or subsequent recall ( Mamiya et al., 2009). Instrumental conditioning of lever pressing for food reward shows a similar pattern ( Izaki et al., 2000; Ostlund and Balleine, 2005) over as does object recognition ( Akirav and Maroun, 2006; Ennaceur et al., 1997). The framework presented here predicts that mPFC will be involved in initial acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval of context-event-response associations. Hence, it cannot fit these data without additional stipulations. One possible explanation is that other prefrontal cortical areas can compensate

for mPFC loss during learning but not during consolidation or recall. For example, aspects of odor-reward association may be mediated by both the OFC as well as ventral mPFC. If ventral mPFC is off-line during learning, OFC may become more heavily involved to the point that it can support learning independently. If mPFC is on-line during learning, however, the mPFC remains essential for consolidation and recall. Numerous research paradigms implicate mPFC in short-term memory, operationally defined here as memory spanning seconds to minutes. Historically, considerable emphasis has been placed on the role of mPFC in memory spanning intervals less than a minute, a capacity referred to as “working” memory (Uylings et al.

Therefore, mSYD1A is a functional Rho-GAP whereas amino acid subs

Therefore, mSYD1A is a functional Rho-GAP whereas amino acid substitutions present in the invertebrate SYD-1 proteins render the GAP domain inactive. Interestingly, the mammalian SYD1A GAP activity is regulated through intra-molecular interactions. Deletion of the intrinsically disordered domain (IDD) and C2 domain resulted in a doubling of mSYD1A GAP activity (Figures 3D–3F). A similar increase was observed when full-length mSYD1A was targeted to

the plasma membrane with an N-terminal lipid modification (myr-mSYD1A) suggesting that full-length learn more mSYD1A is in an autoinhibitory conformation and can be activated by the displacement of N-terminal sequences (Figure 3E). When we coexpressed IDD and GAP domains as independent polypeptides (Figure 3G), the

IDD alone as well as the IDD-C2 domain supplied in cis where able to repress activity of the isolated mSYD1A GAP domain. Finally, we tested whether the inhibition of mSYD1A GAP activity is mediated through protein-protein interactions between the IDD and GAP domains in coimmunoprecipitation experiments ( Figure 3H). Myc-tagged GAP domain was co-immunoprecipitated with the HA-tagged IDD-C2 selleck chemicals domain. Thus, the mSYD1A GAP activity is regulated through protein-protein interactions with the intrinsically disordered N-terminal domain. This suggests that full-length mSYD1A adopts a closed, autoinhibited conformation. Displacement of the IDD, either by truncation or membrane targeting, provides a mechanism for local activation of mSYD1A GAP activity. We tested the functional relevance of the mSYD1A subdomains in synapse formation using gain-of-function experiments. Overexpression of full-length mSYD1A in cultured granule cells

resulted in a 64% ± 10% elevation in the density of synaptic vesicle clusters and a 38% ± 11% increase in synapse density, defined as puncta containing the markers synaptophysin and PSD95. Thus, presynaptic overexpression of mSYD1A is sufficient to stimulate pre- and postsynaptic differentiation. Surprisingly, a mSYD1A mutant lacking the arginine finger (ΔYRL) lost the ability to recruit the postsynaptic marker PSD95 but retained the ability to elevate presynaptic terminal number (Figure 4B). Moreover, a membrane-targeted form of the IDD Astemizole (that lacks the entire C2 and GAP domain sequences of mSYD1A) was sufficient to increase presynaptic terminal density and partially colocalized with the synaptic vesicle marker vGluT1 in axons (Figures 4A and 4B). Importantly, this function of the IDD was also observed when the protein was expressed in neurons lacking full-length mSYD1A expression ruling out an indirect effect through modification of the endogenous protein (Figure S4A). Thus, the IDD is sufficient to drive recruitment of synaptic vesicles independently of the mSYD1A GAP activity.

Figures 5F and 5G show experiments in which Ca2+ waves were evoke

Figures 5F and 5G show experiments in which Ca2+ waves were evoked at various cortical sites by local optogenetic stimulation. In the experiment shown in Figure 5F, the optogenetic stimulation of the visual cortex evoked Ca2+ wave activity that was recorded in the visual, frontal, and contralateral frontal cortices at an increasing latency. Similar results were obtained in six out of six experiments. Figure 5G provides quantitative information on the latencies of Ca2+ wave initiation and propagation to various cortical locations. Thus, the optogenetic stimulation of V1, with a 50 ms light pulse, evoked a Ca2+ wave in V1 within 90 ± 4 ms (14 animals)

(Figure 5GI), the time period required for the local buildup of wave activity. Stimulation in V1 generated a Ca2+ wave http://www.selleckchem.com/products/BIBW2992.html in the ipsilateral FC after 172 ± 5 ms (9 animals) (Figure 5GII) and in the contralateral FC after 204 ± 8 ms (6 animals) (Figure 5GIII). Similar latencies were noted when Ca2+ waves were recorded in V1 upon optogenetic stimulation in the ipsilateral FC (Figure 5GV) or the contralateral FC (Figure 5GVI). Together, these results indicate that different neocortical regions, including V1, FC, and possibly all other cortical areas, can generate Ca2+ waves that can recruit remote cortical sites. To test that the locally

PLX4032 evoked population Ca2+ transient is indeed a propagating wave, we devised a high-speed camera-based approach to record fluorescence signals from large cortical areas (Figure 6A). By multiple injections 17-DMAG (Alvespimycin) HCl of OGB-1, we first stained a larger cortical area with dimensions of about 1–2 by 4–5 mm (Figures 6B and 6C). We then monitored changes in Ca2+ concentration that

occurred at the cortical surface by imaging at 125 frames/s. We found that visual stimulation produced a Ca2+ signal that emerged locally at the visual cortical surface and then gradually propagated toward the frontal cortex (Figure 6D). Propagation in other directions within the skull-covered cortex most likely also took place but could not be monitored. The “wave front” of the Ca2+ transient usually did not form a crisp border but often consisted of active hotspots, indicating that local sites of increased activity preceded the main Ca2+ wave. This notion is also supported by the observation that the rise times of the Ca2+ transients were relatively slow, ranging between 100–200 ms (Figure 6E). The superposition of the Ca2+ transients recorded in the posterior and the anterior portion of the cortex, respectively, indicates the latency of wave occurrence at the remote cortical site (Figure 6E). From such latencies we calculated the speed of Ca2+ wave propagation (Figure 6F) and found that, on average, Ca2+ waves propagated at 37 ± 2 mm/s (105 waves, 5 animals).

To this point, recent work by Luthi and colleagues has shown that

To this point, recent work by Luthi and colleagues has shown that there are anatomically distinct populations of neurons in the basolateral amygdaloid nucleus that respond when animals express either express or suppress conditional fear (Herry et al., 2008). “Fear” neurons responded to nonextinguished CSs or extinguished CSs presented outside the extinction context, whereas extinction

neurons only responded to extinguished CSs presented in their extinction context. Interestingly, the majority of fear neurons were orthodromically activated by electrical stimulation of the ventral hippocampus, whereas extinction neurons received their afferent input from the vmPFC. Hence, the contextual retrieval of fear memory might involve VX-809 datasheet a hippocampo-prefrontal

cortical network that regulates the balance of excitation and inhibition in the amygdala to foster or suppress, respectively, fear to an extinguished CS (Maren, 2005). It is also conceivable that the balance of activity among inhibitory CEl neurons that are either excited (“CS on” neurons) or inhibited (“CS off” neurons) by a CS (e.g., Ciocchi et al., 2010) regulates the suppression or renewal, respectively, of fear after extinction; find more this possibility has not yet been explored. Reducing the expression of fear memory with extinction procedures, such as exposure therapy, is fundamental to therapeutic interventions for fear and anxiety disorders in humans. Unfortunately, the suppression of conditional responding that follows extinction is transient (Bouton, 1993, Bouton and Bolles, 1979a and Rescorla, 2004). In his early work, Pavlov noted that an extinguished CR would return if the animal was presented with a novel stimulus, a phenomenon termed “disinhibition” (Pavlov, 1927). He also showed that extinguished CRs would spontaneously return with the mere passage of time, a phenomenon termed “spontaneous

recovery.” As previously described, extinguished CRs are also highly specific to the experimental context in which they are acquired. In other words, an extinguished CR exhibits “renewal” when the CS is presented outside the extinction context. Similarly, unsignaled USs can restore extinguished responding when the CS is presented in the context in which the US was delivered. This phenomenon is whatever termed “reinstatement” (Bouton and Bolles, 1979b and Rescorla and Heth, 1975). These phenomena indicate that extinction does not erase the conditioning memory, rather it causes new learning about the CS. Indeed, it appears that extinction training yields a new “safety” memory that inhibits retrieval of the fear memory. Unlike fear memory, the expression of this safety memory is limited by context and time (Bouton, 1993). “Context” is defined broadly to include the experimental environment and interoceptive state of the animal, as well as the actual (time of day) and relative time (how long ago) the events were learned.

Type I neuroblasts express Deadpan (Dpn), a bHLH protein related

Type I neuroblasts express Deadpan (Dpn), a bHLH protein related to the vertebrate Hes family, and segregate the homeodomain transcription factor, Prospero (Pros; the ortholog of vertebrate Prox1), to their differentiating daughters. Mapping Prospero’s targets throughout the genome has shown that Prospero directly binds and represses neuroblast genes and cell-cycle genes and is required to activate differentiation genes ( Choksi et al., 2006). As a result, GMCs divide only once to produce two postmitotic neurons or glial cells. By contrast, type II neuroblasts, of which there exist only eight per brain lobe, divide to give a neuroblast and a transit-amplifying GSK1120212 mouse cell called an intermediate neural progenitor

(INP) (Bayraktar et al., 2010, Bello et al., 2008, Boone and Doe, 2008, Bowman et al., 2008 and Weng

et al., 2010). Type II neuroblasts express Deadpan, but not Prospero, and their daughters (INPs) lack Prospero protein. Furthermore, Asense (Jarman et al., 1993), a basic-helix-loop-helix (bHLH) protein and homolog of the vertebrate Veliparib solubility dmso neural stem cell factor Ascl1 (Mash1), is expressed in most larval brain neuroblasts but is markedly absent from type II neuroblasts and immature INPs, which undergo multiple cell divisions (Bayraktar et al., 2010, Bowman et al., 2008 and Weng et al., 2010). Misexpression of Ase appears to be sufficient to transform type II into type I neuroblasts (Bowman et al., 2008). INPs divide from four to eight times, generating another INP and a GMC that divides only once (Figure 1). As a result of the self-renewing divisions of the INPs,

type II neuroblasts generate much larger cell lineages than type I neuroblasts. Despite the differences in lineage output size, the division patterns of type I and type II neuroblasts are both similar to those seen in the mammalian cerebral cortex: apical stem cells in the cortex divide to generate another apical stem cell and either a neuron or a basal progenitor cell, with the latter typically dividing once to generate two postmitotic neurons (Figure 2) (Haubensak et al., 2004, Miyata et al., 2004 and Noctor et al., 2004). The third type of neuroblast is found in the optic lobe of the larval brain, where neural stem cells divide symmetrically within a pseudostratified neuroepithelium and are gradually converted to asymmetrically Sitaxentan dividing neuroblasts in response to a wave of proneural gene expression (Egger et al., 2007, Egger et al., 2011, Hofbauer and Campos-Ortega, 1990 and Yasugi et al., 2008). Again, there are striking parallels here with cortical apical progenitor cells, which form a polarized pseudostratified neuroepithelium and generate neurogenic basal progenitor cells that exit the pseudostratified neuroepithelium (Noctor et al., 2004). During embryogenesis, neuroblasts can be identified by their unique combination of gene expression pattern and time and place of birth.

The precise mechanisms behind the generation of time fields and w

The precise mechanisms behind the generation of time fields and whether other structures organize according to the time kept in the hippocampus remain to be seen,

but Kraus et al. (2013) make it clear that time and place coexist in the hippocampus. D.C.R. is supported by the Marie Curie Foundation (GA-2011-301674). M.B.M. is supported by the Kavli Foundation and a Centre of Excellence grant from the Norwegian Research Council. “
“Spontaneous or endogenously driven neural activity has been a focus of investigation in electrophysiology Ferroptosis assay for many decades (Buzsáki, 2009). In recent years, researchers have focused on fluctuations in blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activity acquired during a “task-free” or “resting” state, as the spatiotemporal structure of these signals has proven richly informative about the functional organization of the human brain (Raichle, 2011). Resting-state dynamics are commonly characterized via “functional connectivity,” which describes the statistical dependence of activity

at different locations in the brain. Resting-state functional connectivity is often computed via a Pearson correlation of fMRI BOLD signal time series recorded from different LBH589 in vitro voxels. Despite the unconstrained mental state in resting-state fMRI experiments, patterns of functional connectivity across the brain are quite reproducible within individuals and across large cohorts of participants (Biswal et al., 2010). This observation suggests that functional connectivity may be shaped by the underlying anatomical connectivity. This notion has gained support from direct

comparisons of anatomical and functional connectivity Org 27569 in the monkey (Vincent et al., 2007) and human (Honey et al., 2009) brain, as well as from interventional studies demonstrating changes in functional connectivity after manipulations of the anatomical substrate (Johnston et al., 2008). In addition, computational models combining cellular biophysics and networks of synaptic connections can generate realistic functional connectivity patterns (Deco et al., 2011). Despite the growing promise of BOLD functional connectivity, important questions remain concerning the optimal data acquisition and analysis methods (Cole et al., 2010) and the spatiotemporal scales at which dynamical correlations usefully indicate functional properties of the brain. Does functional connectivity recorded with fMRI (a slow and indirect neural observation) relate to functional connectivity recorded more directly with invasive electrophysiological methods? Does anatomical connectivity predict resting-state BOLD functional connectivity at spatial scales finer than a cubic millimeter? Can patterns of correlation in the BOLD signal reveal intra-areal functional topographies? In this issue of Neuron, Wang et al. (2013) make significant progress toward addressing these questions. Their focus is on connectivity within area 3b and area 1 of the squirrel monkey somatosensory cortex.

All participants received monetary compensation at a departmental

All participants received monetary compensation at a departmental

standard rate. Participants in the second experiment click here also received a small monetary bonus based on task performance. An MR-compatible joystick (MagConcept, Redwood City, CA) was used. The task was identical to the one used in the EEG experiment, with the following exceptions. For the first experiment initial positions of the icons were randomly assigned to the screen respecting a minimal distance of 150 pixels between icons. For the second experiment initial positions of the icons were rotations or reflections, varied randomly, of a preestablished arrangement of icons of a predetermined triangle with vertices truck (0, 200), package (151, −165), and house (0, −200) (coordinates are in pixels, referenced to the center of the screen). On type D jumps, the destination of the package was chosen randomly

from all locations satisfying the conditions that they (1) increase truck-to-package distance, but (2) leave total path length to the goal (house) unchanged. The forced delay involved in the task interruption (tone, package flashing) totaled 900 ms. At the completion of each delivery, the message “Congratulations!” was displayed for 1000 ms (Figure S1D), followed by a fixation cross that remained on screen for 6000 ms. The first fMRI experiment consisted selleck of three parts: a 15 min behavioral practice

outside the scanner, an 8 min practice inside the scanner during structural scan acquisition, and a third phase of approximately 45 min, where functional data were collected. During functional scanning, 90 trials were completed, in 6 runs of 15 trials each. At the beginning and end of each run, a central fixation cross was displayed for 10,000 ms. The average run length was 7.5 min (range 5.7–11). The task and procedure in the second fMRI experiment were identical to those in the first, with the following exceptions. Type D jumps were replaced almost with type C jumps (see Figure 2 in the main text). In these cases, the distance between truck and package always decreased to 120 pixels. The message “10¢” appeared for 500 ms, indicating the bonus earned for that trial. Immediately following this, a fixation cross appeared for 2500 ms, followed by onset of the next trial. The average run length was 6.8 min (range 4.7–10.7). Image acquisition protocols were the same for both experiments. Data were acquired with a 3 T Siemens Allegra (Malvern, PA) head-only MRI scanner, with a circularly polarized head volume coil. High-resolution (1 mm3 voxels) T1-weighted structural images were acquired with an MP-RAGE pulse sequence at the beginning of the scanning session.

Electronic chips have been placed both epiretinally and subretina

Electronic chips have been placed both epiretinally and subretinally in retinal degeneration patients (Figure 1C). Because they take advantage of the additional processing and cellular connections of the inner retinal neurons, subretinal application of the electronic chips would be expected to provide even more visual detail than epiretinal placement

(Figure 1C). Two subretinally applied chips, the ARGUS II (Second Sight Medical Products) and the Alpha IMS (Retina Implant AG), have a fair amount of human patient experience and are in clinical trial. Steps continue to be taken to improve these devices (for example, to develop wireless power transmission GSI-IX supplier and to develop higher resolution chips). Finally, there is a great deal of interest in stem cell approaches (Ong and da Cruz, 2012). Human stem cells have the potential to develop into a variety of different cell types including photoreceptors or RPE cells. A phase 1 clinical trial is in the process

of evaluating effects of transplantation of human embryonic stem cells into the subretinal space (Figure 1C). Additional preclinical studies aim to evaluate the potential of induced pluripotent stem cells to engraft, differentiate, and restore ABT-263 price function in the diseased retina. Like the other approaches, there are considerable challenges with stem cell delivery. Will the properties of the cells change over time, will they remain localized or spread to undesirable locations, will there be a harmful immune others response, and, in the case of cells destined to become photoreceptors,

will they synapse appropriately with target cells? The passive delivery method for the AAQ compound provides many potential benefits over alternative methods, though there are benefits and drawbacks to all of the above strategies. The utility of the AAQ approach may, however, also extend to organ systems outside of the eye. The transparency of the cornea, media, and retina make it possible to move quickly with the development of retinal therapeutics that harness light-activatable drugs or components, but the AAQ-mediated light activation of other neurons may soon be within our reach. Particular wavelengths of light can penetrate millimeters of skin, for example, and it may be possible to target neurons that control response to touch, temperature, itch, or pain through photochemical therapy. Other organs readily accessible to light through fiber optic technology may be targets as well. With more invasive procedures, it may ultimately be possible to alter broader neurologic functions, such as circadian rhythm and behavior, through photochemical approaches.

To see how these structural disruptions in the mature niche may a

To see how these structural disruptions in the mature niche may affect SVZ neurogenesis, we performed whole-mount IHC staining using antibodies against DCX, Ank3, and acetylated tubulin. We used coordinate-stitching confocal software to acquire Z stack images over the entire ventricular surface, which allowed us to simultaneously assess Ank3/multicilia status and their relationships to newborn neuroblasts traveling in chains beneath the ventricular surface. Confocal images of DCX staining from control P28

mouse ventricular surface revealed robust migratory chains of neuroblasts (Figure 7A). In contrast, iKO mice injected with tamoxifen at P14 and sacrificed at P28 showed significant defects in the coverage of neuroblast chains along the ventricular wall www.selleckchem.com/autophagy.html (Figures 7B and 7C). Since the Foxj1-CreERt2-targeting

Galunisertib nmr strategy generated mosaic populations of mutant and unaffected ependymal cells, we were able to largely avoid the appearance of hydrocephalus harvesting brains 2 weeks after tamoxifen injection (Figure 7B). In some animals we did observe hydrocephalus, as indicated by the enlargement of ventricular surface during tissue harvesting, and this phenotype correlated with extensive removal of ependymal Ank3 expression as confirmed by IHC staining and confocal analysis (Figures 7C and 7D). We inverted the dark-field whole-mount DCX neuroblast images and noted in red, areas where we observed continuous patches of Ank3 defects (accompanying Figures 7B and 7C). After analysis in several tamoxifen-injected iKO mice, we could not find intact DCX+ migratory chains in areas that showed extensive ependymal Ank3 loss (Figures 7B and 7C and data not shown). We observed that on the borders between unaffected ependymal regions and cells with depleted Ank3 expression, DCX+ neuroblast chains became disrupted (Figure 7D and Figure S8D). Predictably, these defects along the ventricular wall led to significant decrease in cellularity/size of the rostral migratory stream in P28 OBs after P14 tamoxifen induction (Figure 7E). It is

interesting to note that 2 weeks after tamoxifen injection, Ank3 expression was often more affected from Foxj1 deletion than surface multicilia, perhaps reflecting the relative turnover rates of each in mature ependymal cells (Figure 7D and Figure S8C). Oxymatrine Consistent with the dramatic reduction in DCX+ neuroblasts, Ki67 staining on coronal sections where large areas of ependyma were targeted showed decreased SVZ proliferation (Figure S8E). To understand whether the iKO phenotypes may be partly due to inducible targeting of SVZ NSCs, we performed lineage-tracing experiments in foxj1-CreERt2; r26r-tdTomato mice. We reasoned that if Foxj1-CreERt2 can mediate significant recombination in mature SVZ NSCs after niche formation, we should see tdTomato+ lineage-traced neuroblast chains along the ventricular wall.